Introduction

The tropics are home to thousands of ant species, some forming colonies containing millions of ants. Collectively, their biomass exceeds the biomass of all wild mammals combined (Bar-On et al. 2018) as they fulfill vital ecological roles as predators, herbivores or soil architects (Hölldobler and Wilson 1990; Davidson et al. 2003). Competition for food sources among colonies can be intense, but intra- and interspecific competition is often reduced through a variety of mechanisms (Hölldobler 1981). Often species occupy particular dietary niches, e.g. by exploiting different food types (sugars, carrion, excrements, seeds, and live prey) or food sizes (Torres 1984; Houadria et al. 2015). Additionally, species can show temporal activity patterns that allow them to avoid competitors (Torres 1984; Stuble et al. 2013; Houadria et al. 2016; Rosumek 2017). For instance, nocturnal species like some Myrmecia or Polyrhachis ants have adapted to low light levels by developing large eyes and efficient navigational strategies (Narendra et al. 2013a, b, 2017). On the other hand, activity patterns of tropical species are also dependent on moisture gradients (Kaspari and Weiser 2000).

Despite these mechanisms of niche differentiation, competition cannot be avoided entirely. The foraging strategies used by the different species are likely to have a strong impact on the ability of colonies to monopolize and exploit food sources fast and efficiently (Traniello 1987, 1989a; Lach et al. 2010; Drescher et al. 2011; Grevé et al. 2019). In particular, communication and recruiting other nestmates to food sources should increase a colony’s chances of displacing competitors and gaining long-term access to food.

Communication among nestmates is widespread in ants and other social insects (Beckers et al. 1989; Hölldobler and Wilson 1990; Jarau and Hrncir 2009; Almeida et al. 2018; Glaser and Grüter 2018; Grüter and Czaczkes 2019). It allows the members of a colony to exchange information about their environment, such as the locations of food sources or the presence of predators and competitors (Kendal et al. 2004; Dawson and Chittka 2014; Lanan 2014). During communication, ants may use tactile signals (e.g. antennation, drumming or vibrations (Hölldobler 1999; Franklin 2014)) or chemical signals (e.g. alarm or trail pheromones (Hölldobler 1976, Traniello 1989b, Lach et al. 2010; Czaczkes et al. 2015)). Ant species with large colony sizes often use chemical mass-recruitment with short- or long-lasting trails, trunk trails, and mass-raids (Beckers et al. 1989, Traniello 1989a, Hölldobler and Wilson 1990, Lanan 2014; Czaczkes et al. 2015). Species with smaller colonies use recruitment strategies like tandem running or group recruitment (Möglich et al. 1974; Beckers et al. 1989; Liefke et al. 2001).

In tandem running, an informed ant returns to the nest after finding a good nest site or a rewarding food source that is too large to be exploited by a single ant. In the nest, she releases a pheromone to attract a potential recruit (Möglich et al. 1974). When the pair walks towards the resource, the follower antennates the leader’s gaster and hind legs to maintain cohesion (Franklin 2014). Although ants in a tandem run-walk slower than ants walking alone, tandem recruitment could save time if resources are hard to find by independent search (Franks and Richardson 2006). Many species (e.g. in Temnothorax or Pachycondyla) use tandem running for both nest emigrations and foraging recruitment (Colin et al. 2017; Grüter et al. 2018), whereas others (e.g. Neoponera or Diacamma) perform tandem runs only during colony migrations, but not during foraging (Fresneau 1985; Kaur et al. 2017). This raises the question whether and how tandem running might improve foraging success and, in particular, whether tandem recruitment might improve foraging success when the competition is intense.

Tropical ants with small colony sizes might be particularly prone to direct competition, so-called interference competition, because they have to compete with mass-recruiting species that use pheromone trails to recruit large numbers of foragers and soldiers that are specialized to fight (Dejean et al. 2005; Czaczkes et al. 2011, 2015; Czaczkes and Ratnieks 2012). It seems highly plausible that communication helps mass-recruiting colonies to monopolize food sources, but it remains unclear whether this is also the case in species with small colonies that use slower forms of communication, such as tandem running. So far, only very few studies have assessed the role of communication in foraging competition in ants. One such study by Dejean et al. (2005) found that Pheidole megacephala (Fabricius, 1793) increases recruitment when they perceive competitors near food sources. Most ponerine ants live in small colonies (often just a couple of hundred individuals or less), they are important generalist arthropod predators and show a diversity of foraging strategies (Nascimento et al. 2012; Schmidt 2013), one of which is tandem recruitment (Grüter et al. 2018).

Here, we tested if recruitment communication helps colonies to be more successful in a highly competitive tropical environment. As a model system, we studied Pachycondyla harpax (Fabricius, 1804), a common Neotropical species that usually uses tandem running to recruit nestmates to newly discovered food sources (Grüter et al. 2018). A previous study observed that P. harpax foragers are often displaced at food sources by aggressive competitors, mainly Pheidole (Grüter et al. 2018). First, we described and quantified the main competitors of P. harpax in a Brazilian environment. Second, we tested our main prediction that tandem recruitment helps colonies to access food sources that would otherwise be monopolized by competitors. Third, we tested if P. harpax might follow a strategy of temporal and spatial niche differentiation to avoid competition, e.g. by foraging in locations and during times when competition for food is lower.

Materials and methods

Study site and study species

Experiments were performed in March 2018 and February/March 2019 on the campus of the University of São Paulo in Ribeirão Preto, Brazil. Our study species Pachycondyla harpax forages for dead and living insects and plant seeds (Grüter et al. 2018). If scouts detect food items, they cannot carry home by themselves, e.g. beetle larvae or caterpillars, they often initiate tandem runs to recruit nestmates to the food source. This species is common in the study area and nests underground (Grüter et al. 2018). Colony sizes range from 15 to 100 individuals that aggressively defend food sources against many smaller ants (Wheeler 1900).

Experimental setup and procedure

All experiments were conducted in the field in three different locations on the campus, where P. harpax occurred. We located and marked colonies by following foragers who return to their nest after offering them small pieces of cheese (mozzarella or “queijo minas”).

Part 1: interspecific competition depending on food types, daytime and territory

We first attempted to identify competitors of P. harpax and explored whether our focal species has a preference for different baits. We consider other ant species that forage at the same time and collect the same food type to be competitors. This does not imply aggressive interactions, i.e. interference competition since also non-aggressive species can reduce the foraging success of focal colonies via exploitation competition (Human and Gordon 1996; Lach 2005). Furthermore, we checked if P. harpax face different levels of competition at different food types and distances from the colony entrance. We tested 15 colonies with four different food sources: we used honey and pieces of mango (approx. 1 cm3) as carbohydrate baits and cheese cubes (approx. 1 cm3) or meat (pieces of sausage or mealworms) as protein-rich food sources. Food baits were placed at two different distances from the colony entrances: either at 30 cm (“inner territory”) or at 100 cm (“outer territory”). Previous research has shown that most food items are collected < 100 cm from the nest entrance (Grüter et al. 2018).

Furthermore, we tested if P. harpax face stronger competition during the daytime or at night. We offered food baits (cubes of cheese, approx. 1 cm3) during the morning (9.00–12.00 h) and after sunset (18.30–21.00 h). Due to the low activity of P. harpax in the afternoon, we did not test for competition during that time. We again tested the influence of distance from the nest.

After providing a food source, we recorded the number of different ant species (competitors) at the food bait during and at the end of a 10-min observation period. In case an ant species could not be identified by the observer, specimens were collected for later identification. Ants were identified by comparison with the species of the Entomological Collection Padre Jesus Santiago Moure, Universidade Federal do Paraná, Curitiba, Brazil (DZUP), where the vouchers were deposited.

Part 2: exploitation and recruitment depending on competition

We provided food baits to 42 individual foragers of P. harpax (31 colonies). After they accepted the food and, thus, were the first species at the food source (treatment bait, without competition at the beginning), we provided a second food source as a control bait approx. 30 cm from the first food source and at a similar distance to the nest. The second food source was normally discovered quickly by competitors but could also be found by P. harpax. The second bait allowed us to perform a paired comparison between the two baits near a colony. As soon as the food sources were placed, we started filming (JVC GZ-GX1 camcorder) the immediate surroundings of the two food baits. In some trials, we made still images of the control bait. We recorded at least one image per minute. Additionally, both baits were regularly inspected. Each trial was filmed for approx. 60 min. Observations ended earlier if other ant species displaced P. harpax from the first bait. It was not possible to quantify the number of food that was removed from the bait without disturbing the experiments. We frequently observed single foragers, to breaking-off bits and to walk away and transporting it back to the nest.

During video analysis, we recorded the distance of each bait from the nest (whenever possible), the probability of recruitment by P. harpax colonies, the maximum number of P. harpax foragers at the bait (at the same time), whether and for how long P. harpax foragers had access to the bait, the time until competitors found the bait and if there was a takeover (within 60 min after initial discovery P. harpax did not have access anymore) by competitors. P. harpax foragers were considered to have access to the food source (yes or no) when the individuals of a colony had access to the food source for at least 15 min and were able to collect and exploit the food bait, even if there were other non-aggressive ant species present. We considered a takeover by another ant species to have occurred when P. harpax could no longer feed at the bait.

Part 3: daily activity of Pachycondyla harpax

We observed that P. harpax appeared to be more active after sunset (unpublished data; see also García-Pérez et al. 1997). To confirm this, we observed 15 colonies at various locations on the campus during several days (14 March 2019–21 March 2019), from 9.30 h in the morning until 20.00 h in the evening. Each colony was observed for one minute and all P. harpax individuals in an area of one meter around the nest entrance were recorded. If we found no focal ant, the colony was considered to be inactive. After sunset, we explored the surrounding of the nest entrance using the light of headlamps.

Part 4: food exploitation success depending on daytime

To compare the foraging success of P. harpax during the daytime (9.30–12.00 h) and after sunset (18.30–21.00 h), we tested 15 colonies in both time periods and provided cheese baits for individual P. harpax foragers. Every five minutes during a 45-min observation period we performed a scan-sampling and checked whether foragers from the focal colony still had access and recorded the maximum number of foragers that were simultaneously present at the food source. This allowed us to explore the competitive performance of colonies during the day and at night.

Statistical analysis

All tests were performed in R 3.5.3 (R Development Core Team 2019). We performed linear mixed-effect models (LMEs) for normally distributed response variables and generalized linear mixed-effect models (GLMMs) for response variables with a binomial or Poisson distribution (Zuur et al. 2009). For the competition and food exploitation experiments, colony ID was used as a random effect to account for the potential non-independence of observations from the same colony. Location was used as a random factor for the recruitment experiments (Zuur et al. 2009). For the activity analyzes, we used colony and date as random factors. In part 1, we tested the role of the fixed effects “daytime”, “colony territory” (inner vs. outer) and “food type” on the presence of competitors and P. harpax activity. In part 2, to test the influence of recruitment on the foraging success, we used the fixed effects “maximum number of P. harpax foragers at the baits” and “recruitment” (yes or no) to test if they predicted whether P. harpax had access (yes or no) to the food bait. Since these predictors are linked, we explored their importance separately. With a McNemar’s test we analyzed if the probability to recruit was higher at the treatment than at the control feeder. To test for significant interactions among the fixed-effects, we used likelihood ratio tests (LRT) and compared the model without the interaction with the model containing the interaction. Non-significant interactions were removed from the model. In part 3, we tested if colony activity (ants vs. no ants) depended on the time of the day. In part 4, we quantified the foraging activity of focal colonies depending on the time interval during the 45-min observation period.

Results

Interspecific competition depending on food types and territory

Overall, we identified almost 40 different ant species at our baits during the 10-min observation periods (Table 1). The most common competitors of P. harpax were mass-recruiting Pheidole species (mostly Pheidole oxyops Forel 1908), Odontomachus sp.; rarely, we found Atta or Camponotus at a food bait (Fig. 1). A similar number of ant species discovered the different types of food during the 10-min observation period, irrespective of the distance from the P. harpax focal colony (range = 30–100 cm, 2.68 ± 1.20 species [mean ± sd]). There was a borderline significant difference between the total number of species that discovered honey compared to meat, with more species collecting honey than meat (GLMER: inner vs. outer territory: z = − 0.792, p = 0.428; food sources: meat vs. honey: z = 2.080, p = 0.038; meat vs fruit: z = 0.816, p = 0.415; meat vs. cheese: z = 0.931, p = 0.352; cheese vs. honey: z = 1.190, p = 0.234; cheese vs. fruit: z = − 0.095, p = 0.924; fruit vs. honey: z = 1.252, p = 0.211). At the end of the 10-min observation period, the number of ant species at the food baits decreased to 1.36 ± 0.76 species and there was no difference between food types (GLMER: inner vs. outer territory: z = − 0.643, p = 0.520; food sources: meat vs honey: z = 1.500, p = 0.134; meat vs fruit: z = − 0.067, p = 0.947; meat vs. cheese: z = 0.420, p = 0.675; cheese vs. honey: z = 1.104, p = 0.269; cheese vs. fruit: z = − 0.479, p = 0.125; fruit vs. honey: z = 1.536, p = 0.211). This reduction is likely due to the displacement of weakly competitive species by competitively superior species.

Table 1 Competing ant species observed at different food baits during our observations
Fig. 1
figure 1

Study species and competitors at food baits. a Pachycondyla harpax defending the food source against Wasmannia auropunctata. b Atta sexdens picking up and carrying away the cheese bait. c Workers and soldiers of mass recruiting Pheidole oxyops (Photo by Tomer Czaczkes). d Odontomachus chelifer carrying away a small piece of cheese

Exploitation and recruitment depending on competition

We found that after offering P. harpax foragers a bait, a substantial proportion of ants initiated tandem recruitment. In 42.9% (18 out of 42) of all trials, at least one forager started recruiting. The control bait was found in 38.1% (16 out of 42) of all trials by at least one P. harpax forager. Tandem recruitment to the control bait occurred in 37.5% (6 out of 16) of these trials (14.3% of all trials). Overall, tandem recruitment by P. harpax foragers was significantly more likely at the treatment bait than at the control bait (McNemar’s test: χ2 = 6.050, df = 1, p = 0.014). The distance between the nest and the food source did not influence recruitment probability (GLMER: z = 1.052, p = 0.293). When comparing the number of P. harpax foragers at the two baits, we found no correlation between the maximum number of ants at the treatment and control feeder (GLMER: z = − 0.653, p = 0.514). A positive correlation might have been expected if the number of ants at the two paired baits would have been the result of the colony size of the focal colony or reflected the level of competition in the area of the focal colony. Furthermore, competitors found the treatment and control feeder equally fast (treatment: 1.66 ± 2.28 min vs. control: 2.05 ± 1.79 min (mean ± sd); GLMER: z = − 1.318, p = 0.187) and there was no correlation of time of discovery by competitors (GLMER: z = 1.526, p = 0.127).

We predicted that recruitment would lead to an increase in ants at the bait and, in turn, a higher probability to have access to the food bait. The results show that significantly more ants reached the food source when recruitment took place (Fig. 2a) (GLMER: z = 5.942, p < 0.001). Thus, when ants performed more tandem runs, the maximum number of ants that were present at a bait increased (GLMER: z = 4.486, p < 0.001). During a trial, individual foragers were often seen walking to the nest with small pieces of food and, subsequently, return to the food source in a tandem run or alone. We found that recruitment was associated with a more than 4-times higher probability to have access to the food source (no recruitment: 20.8% vs. recruitment: 88.9%) (Fig. 2b) (GLMER: z = 2.723, p = 0.006). Related to these two findings, we predicted that colonies are more likely to keep access if more ants are at the food source. We found that the probability of having access increased significantly with an increasing number of foragers at the food bait (Fig. 2c) (GLMER: z = 2.723, p = 0.006): P. harpax colonies had a 100% access chance when at least three ants were at the food source.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Maximum number of ants (a) and the probability of ants at the food source (b) depending on whether our focal colony performed tandem recruitment. Probability of access (c) and of a food takeover by a competitive ant species (d) depending on the maximum number of ants at the food source. Access refers to a period of exploitation of at least 15 min during the observation period. Takeovers occurred when focal colonies were displaced during the 60 min observation period. Boxplots show median, 25th and 75th quartile and the 5th and 95th percentile. Bar plots show means and SE. Note that all values in (c) and (d) are either 1 or 0 but that jitter was used to better visualize the data points. Grey areas show the 95% confidence interval. ***p < 0.001

When competitors took over the food source at any time during the 60-min observation period, P. harpax stopped recruiting. We tested if an increasing number of foragers at the food source lowered the chance of a takeover by another species and indeed found this to be the case (GLMER: z = − 3.087, p = 0.002). When there were at least five ants of P. harpax at the food bait, takeover was extremely unlikely (Fig. 2d).

Activity cycle

We found in the first experiments that P. harpax has an increased activity after sunset (~ 6.30 pm) (see also García-Pérez et al. 1997). Therefore, we quantified the activity of 15 colonies from early morning (sunrise ~ 6.15 am) until after sunset (9:30 h to 20:00 h) over eight days (Fig. 3). There was substantial variation in activity throughout the day. As described by García-Pérez et al. (1997) in a different area, activity was generally low during the day and increased in the evening around 17.00 h up to 18.30 h (GLMER: χ2 = 84.133, df = 7, p < 0.001). While observing the colony activity, we also measured the temperature throughout the day. The morning counts ended around 11 am (26.88 °C ± 1.63 °C). The afternoon counts lasted from 12:30 pm until 5 pm (28.70 °C ± 1.72 °C). Sunset was typically around 6:30 pm. This is when our evening measurements started (24.70 °C ± 1.34 °C). The temperature was always measured a few cm above the ground in shaded areas.

Fig. 3
figure 3

The probability of colony activity of P. harpax foragers during daytime over 8 days (n = 15). Sunset was around 18.30

Interspecific competition depending on daytime and territory

One explanation for the increased activity at night could be that colonies face less competition than during the daytime. Thus, we compared the number of ant species at food baits in the morning vs. the evening. Contrary to our expectation, we observed that significantly more competitor species discovered the bait after sunset (Fig. 4a) (LME: t = − 2.355, p = 0.024). There was also a tendency for more ant species to discover the bait closer to the nest entrance (LME: t =− 1.753, p = 0.087).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Number of different competitors (a) and the probability (b) that P. harpax would discover the bait during 10 min depending on the time of day and the distance from the entrance of the focal colony (territory). Sample sizes refer to the number of trials. n.s. not significant, *p < 0.05

The probability that P. harpax foragers would discover the baits during the 10-min observation period was higher at night than during daytime (Fig. 4b) (GLMER: z = − 2.332, p = 0.020). This probability did not depend on the distance of the cheese bait from the nest entrance of the focal colonies (GLMER: z = − 0.354, p = 0.724). There was a borderline non-significant trend that P. harpax were more likely to still have access to the food at night compared to daytime at the end of the 10-min observation period, (GLMER: daytime: z = 1.910, p = 0.056; territory: z = − 0.363, p = 0.716). The most frequent and successful competitor was the mass-recruiting Pheidole oxyops (present in 62.2% experiments) (Fig. 1c).

Food exploitation and access depending on daytime

We then presented individual P. harpax foragers with cheese baits to explore whether time of day affected their ability to maintain access to the food source during a 45-min observation period. During daytime, the number of foragers at the bait remained constant and low, whereas the number of P. harpax at the food increased after sunset (Fig. 5a, b) (GLMER: morning vs. evening: z = − 8.056, p < 0.001; time: morning: χ2 = 10.033, df = 8, p = 0.263; evening: χ2 = 27.66, df = 8, p < 0.001).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Number of individual P. harpax workers and the probability of colony presence at food baits during the 45-min period in the morning (a, c) and evening (b, d). n.s. not significant, ***p < 0.001

Not all colonies were able to keep access to the food bait over the 45 min. After sunset, more colonies kept access to the food source than in the morning (morning: 18.5% vs. evening: 58.6%) (Fig. 5c, 5d (GLMER: morning vs. evening: z = − 8.056, p < 0.001; time: morning: χ2 = 3.2 × 1010, df = 8, p < 0.001; evening: χ2 = 801.82, df = 8, p < 0.001).

Discussion

Our results support the hypothesis that recruitment communication by tandem running increases access to food sources and, thus, foraging success in Pachycondyla harpax. Competition in this Brazilian habitat was intense for all types of food as almost 40 different ant species discovered and exploited our baits during our observations. This intense competition is not surprising given that P. harpax is a generalist forager, like many other Neotropical ant species. Confronted with big food items, foragers attempted to cut off small pieces of food and transport them back to the nest. However, they were frequently displaced by competitor species. Although several different species can discover a food source, only a few of them remain at the food bait over longer time periods. This reduction is likely due to displacement by competitively superior species. The most abundant competitors were Pheidole spp. (Table 1; 94.6% of all observations), which are very efficient and aggressive mass-recruiters (Czaczkes et al. 2011; Czaczkes and Ratnieks 2012). While it was not possible to collect data on the number of competing individuals at baits, the observations on the dominance of Pheidole spp. suggest that the number of competing ants affects the ability of P. harpax to access and defend food items. In some cases, however, a single individual of Odontomachus or Neoponera could banish their competitors, demonstrating that there is not always a straightforward relationship between the number and dominance of competitors. Different competitors could also fight against each other and thereby give a third party the opportunity to have access and exploit a food bait.

It is often assumed that mass-recruitment via pheromone trails helps colonies to monopolize food sources (de Biseau et al. 1997; Detrain and Deneubourg 2008; Drescher et al. 2011). Accordingly, Pheidole megacephala scouts start recruiting more nestmates and soldiers to a food source if pheromones from competing colonies are encountered (Dejean et al. 2005). Our results support and extend this view by showing that also species with small colonies employing relatively slow recruitment mechanisms are likely to benefit from recruitment communication. When P. harpax foragers had an opportunity to recruit nestmates to a food source, their foraging success was much higher: focal colonies with recruitment were 4 times more likely to keep access to the food during our observations. This is likely to allow more foragers to recruit further ants, leading to positive feedback and a larger number of ants at the food. The bait was unlikely to be taken over by competitors when about five or more P. harpax foragers were at the food source (Fig. 2d). When P. harpax foragers found the food source by chance, they were often not able to exploit it for longer time periods if competitors were already present and prevented other P. harpax ants to recruit nestmates. When focal foragers discovered the bait before competitor species, as was the case at the treatment bait, tandem recruitment was much more frequently observed compared to the control bait (43% vs. 14%), which was randomly placed in the vicinity of the same nest. Thus, finding a food source first, before competitor species, is of critical importance to foraging success. The recruitment probability in P. harpax is comparable or higher than in other species that use recruitment methods other than chemical trails. For example, about 10% of returning honeybee foragers perform a waggle dance (Seeley 1995) and ~ 10 to 30% of ant foragers lead tandem runs to food sources in Cardiocondyla venustula and Temnothorax nylanderi (Wilson 1959; Glaser and Grüter 2018).

Competition might be less intense in some areas, allowing more P. harpax foragers to have access for longer and to recruit nestmates to exploit the food source. A large number of P. harpax at a bait might also indicate a large colony size. In both cases, we would expect a positive correlation between the paired treatment and control baits in terms of the number of P. harpax foragers exploiting them. In other words, the two baits in the proximity of a particular nest should be similar in the number of P. harpax foragers. However, this was not the case in our study as the number of P. harpax foragers at the treatment feeder was unrelated to the number at the paired control feeder. Additionally, there was no correlation in how quickly competitors found the treatment and control feeder. This finding, in combination with our other findings, suggests that the number of ants at a bait is largely the result of discovering a food source first, followed by successful recruitment. Colony size might still be an important factor for competitiveness. When a colony is larger, more individuals can scout or be recruited to a food source. Hence, larger colonies are probably more successful during foraging compared to smaller colonies (Dornhaus et al. 2012). In addition, foragers from larger colonies might also be more aggressive (Oster and Wilson 1978), which would further affect access to food sources.

One possibility to avoid competition might be to forage during different times of the day, as is the case with nocturnal and diurnal species (Rosumek 2017). In a study on grassland ants, for example, different species foraged at different times of the day depending on air temperature (Albrecht and Gotelli 2001). This temporal niche partitioning can help subdominant species to avoid dominant species (Stuble et al. 2013). Stingless bees, likewise, shift their activity to avoid the presence of other, mainly aggressive species (Nagamitsu and Inoue 1997, Keppner and Jarau 2016). In the tropics, species also can adapt to different seasons, e.g. by being more active during the rainy seasons (Baumgartner and Roubik 1989), whereas others are active during both seasons. In accordance with a previous study (García-Pérez et al. 1997), we observed that the activity of P. harpax increases in the afternoon and is highest after sunset. Thus, we hypothesized that this shift in activity is due to the competition being less intense after sunset. Contrary to this expectation we found that competition seemed to be even stronger after sunset than during the morning/daytime: we found a 33% increase in the number of competitor species at the food baits after sunset. A possible explanation might be that most ant species start becoming more active when temperatures are going down and humidity increases to prevent water loss (Schilman et al. 2007). The > 100% increase in P. harpax activity after sunset (Fig. 3) more than compensated for the increase in competitor species after sunset (Fig. 4) and, as a result, P. harpax colonies were better able to defend and exploit food sources after sunset (Fig. 5). After 45 min, only 18.5% of the colonies maintained access to the food during the morning compared to 56.6% after sunset. This suggests that P. harpax might be more successful during the night because they increase their activity at a higher rate than other ant species. As a result, we observed an increase in the number of foragers at the food bait over time after sunset, but not during daytime. This increase is most likely due to ants being recruited via tandem runs (personal observation) in combination with a higher rate of independent discoveries at night.

Conclusions

It is often assumed that recruitment communication in social insects is beneficial for foraging success (but see Dechaume-Moncharmont et al. 2005, I’Anson Price et al. 2019). One key benefit could be that recruitment allows colonies to monopolize food sources in a competitive environment by building up a critical mass of nestmates to defend a large resource. However, evidence for this has been scarce. In our study, we show that Pachycondyla harpax is likely to improve their foraging success thanks to tandem recruitment. The results also indicate that foragers have better access to food sources at night, most likely aided by recruiting nestmates. This highlights the potential influence of communication on foraging success and efficiency in ants, including those species with small colony sizes and slow, direct recruitment communication.